Newsgroups: rec.music.compose Path: spud.Hyperion.COM!netcomsv!netcom.com!csus.edu!wupost!uwm.edu!caen!umeecs!zip.eecs.umich.edu!fields From: fields@zip.eecs.umich.edu (Matthew Fields) Subject: GEMS 2 [repost] Message-ID: <1992Nov17.211433.26969@zip.eecs.umich.edu> Sender: news@zip.eecs.umich.edu (Mr. News) Organization: University of Michigan EECS Dept., Ann Arbor, MI Date: Tue, 17 Nov 1992 21:14:33 GMT Lines: 367 GEMS 2 ==== = Matthew H. Fields I mentioned a willingness to post some general and specific observations regarding music composition, and so far, I've received an enthusiastic response. Therefore, this is the second such posting. In my first GEM article (named after the phrase 'gems of wisdom' that was passed around a great deal in the discussion that preceeded the first such posting), I discussed dramatic shape and climax-building, and passed on several famous hints for building better climaxes to dramatic musical works. Today's presentation is a bit more philosophical, and takes a more round-about route towards being helpful to composers. The topic for today is: PARALLEL FIFTHS AND OCTAVES --- WHY I BOTHER ABOUT THEM I have chosen to present this topic here in my sequence because most of my later proposed articles will be written assuming you have some idea of my biases regarding countrapuntal issues. This article will not contain any hints or suggestions regarding composition, but will instead talk about some meta-issues of perception. Disclaimers: I am presenting the material here mainly as my opinion. If you try to make use of my suggestions and they don't help you write fabulous music, I don't accept any liability. Likewise, it is strictly to your credit and none of mine if you do write fabulous music before or after reading these posts. Plenty of the ideas I will be discussing in this series have been mentioned before, and some theorists may even wish to lay copyright claim or patent claim to some of them. However, I claim that the core ideas have been known to composers and used by them long before anybody published any writings on them, and these ideas are therefore basically in the public domain. In fact, some of these ideas have even been bandied about on rec.music.compose in recent weeks, often quite well. On the other hand, I actually sat down and wrote the text of this posting, and it took me a bit of time and thought, so if anybody were to exploit this text as a commodity without consulting me, I might get very mad (standard disclaimer). Furthermore, to the best of my knowledge, at no time have I herein explicitly quoted anybody's special article: this prose is all mine. All that having been said, I am interested in getting some feedback on how interesting or useful you find this article. ABSTRACT In this article, I will describe a perceptual basis for being careful concerning the use of parallel octaves and fifths. I don't expect to convince anybody to take on such a concern, and I most especially will not hand out any rules, generative or proscriptive, on this matter. On the other hand, it is my intent to argue that this concern is not obsolete but current, and not a matter of abstract rule-making, nor a matter of mystical invocation of physics, but rather a matter of hearing and musical expression. INTRODUCTION Parallel octaves and fifths: we hear of a 'proscription' against them in our music theory classes. Then we find out that Bach's organs had 8-foot, 4-foot, and 3-foot stops, so that every melody he played could be sounded out in parallel octaves and fifths. Even worse, we discover that parallel octaves are ubiquitous in ensemble music and piano music. And then, as we delve into musical history, we discover early forms of organum in which singers always sang in parallel fifths. Why, then, is a big deal made about these things in theory classes? and why these intervals, only, and not thirds, sixths, and sevenths? What is the role of dogma and propaganda in this matter? As I so often do -- perhaps it's a Jewish habit? -- I'll begin my answer with a story. No, not "we were slaves in the land of Mitzrayim", but rather: once, I was teaching the rudiments of aural skills to a total beginner, and he was working on the game of "name that interval", meaning that given the sound of two pitches played either sequentially or simultaneously, he was to name the interval between them. He complained at one point that he was having a bit of trouble hearing octaves and fifths when the notes were played simultaneously, and he said it sounded like the upper pitch was somehow 'hiding' behind the lower pitch. I probed him a bit on this observation: had he noticed this phenomenon outside of his work with the aural-skills software? Yes, he had started noticing it in all the music he heard. Did it apply to other intervals? Yes, especially strongly to the unison, and quite weakly to the major third. I was, of course, surprised to hear a beginner mentioning such a phenomenon. He had never heard of any rule which made a big deal about parallel octaves and fifths, and was quite surprised by it when it came up in his theoretical studies---after all, parallel octaves are ubiquitous in piano music. But he was a dilligent student, and promptly proposed an abstract theory in which parallel octaves and fifths were somehow purely timbral events of physics, while other parallel intervals were events of multiple melodies. Many authors continue to describe the harmonic series and say, without further explanation, that it is the cause of the concern with parallel fifths and octaves. I think that such a description of the physical world is not sufficient to describe how certain composers have treated these materials, but coupling that description with some purely /SUBJECTIVE/ observations (like the ones my student complained of) may actually bring us closer to an understanding of the matter. Even that will not be enough to explain the concern with parallelisms, though, since parallelism is a matter of melodic motion, not of how we perceive individual intervals. DEFINITIONS Before I go any further, let's make sure we're all talking about the same things. When I say that two parts are in /unison/, I mean that they are sounding the same pitch at the same time, i.e. in the same octave. For the acoustically-minded out there, this means that within tolerances that our ears define, they are sounding the same fundamental frequency (where applicable). When I say that one note is /an octave higher/ than the other, I mean that it sounds the eighth ascending diatonic step from the other, or is at an ascending distance of seven diatonic steps, or twelve half-steps (in 12-tone equal temperment). For the acoustically- minded, this means tolerably close to a frequency ratio of 2:1, so A-880 is an octave above A-440, and A-1760 is an octave higher than that. Naturally, if I say that a note is an octave /lower/ than a second note, this means just that the second one is an octave higher than the first. Carrying out the arithmetic, we find that the first note is seven diatonic steps below the second note, or twelve half-steps below the second note, or tolerably-close to a frequency ratio of 1:2 with the second. When I say that one note is a perfect fifth higher than another, I mean that there is an ascending distance of 7 half-steps between them. I don't give this definition in diatonic steps, because while the fifth diatonic step in the C-major scale over C is G, at a distance of 7 half-steps, the fifth diatonic step over B is F, at a distance of only 6 half-steps. So, I'm saying that I care about the distance being 7 half-steps, regardless of where it sits in the scale. For the acoustically-minded, the frequency ratio this time is 3:2. In 12-tone equal temperment, this ratio (which can be precisely expressed in decimal form as 1.5) is approximated by the seven-twelveths power of 2 (~~1.498307077, or a little more than 1% flat). Finally, by /compound interval/, I mean an interval augmented by the addition of one or more octave to its distance. In the case of a perfect fifth, the first few compoundments of it are the perfect twelveth and the perfect 19th, or distances of 12+7=19 and 24+7=31 half-steps, or frequency ratios of 3:1 and 6:1 (within tolerances). The tolerances I mention above have been the topic of quite a lot of debate over the years, so I'm not going to pin them down, partly because doing so would not add any vital information to this article. Mathematicians out there are asked to please refrain from the temptation to say 'Let epsilon be any positive real number'. If anybody is tempted to do that, would they please agree that our tolerances are less than 2% of the lower frequency for the sake of this article? Ok. I'm not going to talk about quantitative acoustics much more in this article, because I think it's time to talk about psychological phenomena. SO WHAT'S THE BIG DEAL? All right, we're getting to that. But first, let's talk about melody. I THOUGHT THIS WAS ABOUT PARALLEL FIFTHS. Yes, but we're coming to that, and we have to back up and visit melody and polyphony on the way. A long time ago, somebody first started coming up with the notion of 'a nice melody' or 'a nice melodic shape' that some of us still use today (it's the first thing you now study when you learn species counterpoint). The basics of this concept were things like: it had one and only one climax point, which was typically its highest note, or sometimes its lowest note; it started on, ended on, and generally circled around a main note which was supposed to express a sense of repose; it moved mainly by step, occasionally by third, and rarely by fourth or fifth --- any time a string of notes was constructed that leaped a lot up and down, this was perceived not as a single melody but rather as a sort of time-sharing between two or more melodies, each of which moved stepwise (/compound melody/). Long before people were experimenting with what we now call harmony, they had gotten pretty good at building interesting and exciting things that were single melodic lines. After a while, folks tried two crucial experiments that forever changed the way people made music: 1) Two folks got together and sang the same melody at the same time; 2) Two folks got together and sang different melodies at the same time. Of course, this last sentence is a gross oversimplification of history, and is not a documented event anywhere in the world. But let's consider the consequences of the two experiments anyhow. In the first case, perhaps the people had the same voice range the first time they tried this, in which case they sang in unison, and the sound reverberated larger than either of them. Or perhaps, the first time they tried this, they had such different voice ranges that they sang in octaves (Perhaps an evolutionary theorist could explain our ability to recognize melodic content after transposition in terms of our needing to recognize the same intonation pattern from adults and children?). Now, the first people to try singing two different melodies together had a much more complicated result. Certain combinations of tones came to be called pleasing-sounding, and others, anxious-sounding; from these basic notions, a variety of complex systems of consonance and dissonance were developed---which were different in different eras---and plans were developed for ways in which various consonances and dissonances could be strung together to express something vaguely analogous to a sentence-structure. Meanwhile, folks were listening to, and enjoying, two melodic shapes at once. At one point, the two shapes crossed through the same note, perhaps. The listeners became confused, because just after the crossing, it was hard to tell whether the voices had bounced off each other like this i ---\v/--- i * ii ---/^\--- ii or crossed through each other like this: i ---\ /--- ii X ii ---/ \--- i Some folks complained that trying to keep the melodies clear in their heads detracted from their appreciation of the individual melodies as well as their appreciation of the consonances and dissonances that arose between them. So some musicians tried to find pairs of melodies that eliminated the second possibility altogether, so after a while, everyone would get used to hearing things the first way anyhow. Sooner or later, it was bound to happen: the two melodies passed through two notes in a row exactly the same: ----- i i ---\__ *< _>* \____ ii _/ ii /\/ People had gotten used to keeping the two melodies clear in their heads for one shared note, but two in a row was just too hard for many people. It sounded like one of the melodies had momentarily gone silent while the other had momentarily gotten stronger or louder. At about the same time, ideas of perspective, shadows, and oclusion were being developed in the visual arts, and people had analogous ideas brewing regarding making foreground and background shapes all equally visible and readily enjoyable. So, some musicians decided that in their compositions, one was the largest number of consecutive notes in a row on which two melodic lines would sound in unison, the better to allow the listeners to follow the shapes of each of the lines up and down. But the situation in music was more complex. Some folks, like my talented student, felt a sense of conjunction and aural oclusion at not just the unison, but the octave as well, and its compoundments. These folks decided that when two players were supposed to be playing different musics, they'd never have two consecutive octaves with each other, again so the melodies wouldn't seem to hide one behind the other for too long for their enjoyment of each melodic shape by itself as well as the overall composite. Some folks had the same experience with the fifth and its compoundments, and foreswore parallel fifths from their multiple-melody expression (counterpoint). Perhaps some folks even experienced the same perception with parallel fourths, thirds, and sixths; if so, those folks probably got disgusted with the whole thing and went into something like mathematics or geography, where great new things were being uncovered every day. Meanwhile, the consequences of experiment number 1 above were still brewing. Having worked out several melodies to sound simultaneously, people sometimes had more resources than melodies. They quickly found that two violins playing the same melody could balance one bass or cello playing another melody better than one of each (due to the differences in inherent size and loudness of the instruments). Furthermore, individual melodies could be played by pairs of players playing in octaves, often without changing much about the effect of the music except its perceived loudness and strength. Harpsichord builders and organ builders made automatic doubling at the upper octave a feature of their instruments, essentially a simple way of getting a stronger sound with the same number of perceived melodies. Orchestrators eventually decided on a rule for groups of players, which still seems to work pretty well: octave doubling above the highest melody, and below the lowest melody, but no octave-doubling of inner melodies, as such doubling was perceived as still confusing to the ears---except when it was provided by highly-controlled, automated means, like organ stops, harpsichord stops, or 12-string lutes and guitars. Organs even came to have extra pipes to produce parallel 12ths (compoundments of fifths) for an even brighter, stronger tone. So, for a great deal of western polyphonic (multi-melody) music, parallel octaves and fifths were considered as falling into two categories: features of a single melody--often highly-desireable reinforcements of a melody that contributed to its tone color and perceived loudness; and momentary interactions between two melodies--usually considered undesirable, because they interfered with /some/ listeners' ability to enjoy both melodies to the fullest. Some people continue to hear in these terms, and find ways to treat these 'sensitive' parallelisms as either constant features of their music or things that rarely or never occur in their music. Composers of the classical era worked out some highly elaborate ways of constructing contrapuntal music so that it avoided parallel octaves and fifths---yet didn't sound (to them) highly artificial. The study of the methods and tricks used by these composers (which involved the resolution of a lot of other preferences and conventions as well as the avoidance of or isolation and control of these special parallelisms) eventually blossomed into our modern discipline of classical counterpoint and harmonic theory. This field and course of study is now so loaded with interesting tidbits of musical thought that the concept of parallel octaves and fifths is often dismissed with the shorthand comment "they're forbidden"---occasionally with a brief mention of the harmonic series, or of the vague idea that they interfere with independent motion. But, of course, the truth of the matter is a bit more subtle. LISTENING ASSIGNMENT Once again, the assignments are purely optional. Give serious consideration to playing around with parallel fifths and octaves. Do your ears tell you anything about them? Do you have an attitude about them? How do you perceive music that avoids them? (try the first or second fugue from Book One of the Well-Tempered Clavier of JS Bach) music that uses them constantly? (try the sarabande from Pour le piano by Claude DeBussy) music that uses them indifferently? (supply your own example) music that uses them constantly for long stretches, then not at all, but never indifferently (try the Tenth fugue in e minor from book one of the Well Tempered Clavier of JS Bach) ? See if you can find sources and recordings documenting the effect of different tuning systems on the sound of the music. Do your discoveries suggest anything for your own compositional preferences? WRITTEN ASSIGNMENT No written assignment this time. Go compose. CONCLUSION I hope this article was interesting. In writing it, I've tried to condense an enormous amount of information and ideas into a small space. While the resulting article is still rather long, some of the topics treated--especially the musical history--are quite eliptical, abbreviated, and abstract. However, I hope that for those readers who find the article too hurried in its descriptions, the subject matter may at least be intriguing, and those readers may wish to look into it further, starting perhaps with the New Grove Dictionary of Music and Musicians s.v. /counterpoint/. For at least a while I will be keeping a copy of this article here in my disk directory. As long as the volume of "reprint" requests is reasonably manageable, I will offer to send copies out by e-mail. I can't really tell you when the next article in this series will be ready for posting, since I haven't written it yet. The next article will be aimed at the student enrolled in the typical undergraduate theory course, who has been asked to demonstrate proficiency at 18th- century harmonic counterpoint. It will consist of a very short list of things to try as shortcuts, so that the reader might finish their theory homework earlier and have more time available for composing. 4 September 1992 Matthew H. Fields, D.M.A. .